Many arguments about science are not about science at all.
Sliding vs Deciding: This blog is about romantic relationships and marriage, with insights from relationship science about how relationships develop and what makes or breaks them.
Friday, May 13, 2016
My Shortest Blog Entry Ever
Many arguments about science are not about science at all.
Saturday, May 7, 2016
Searching for “The One”: Mate Selection in this Modern World
by Scott Stanley
In their book Modern Romance, Aziz Ansari and Eric Klinenberg focus on how people search
for a partner, a date, or a mate in this hyper-connected era of having a seemingly
endless number of options. Modern Romance
is not for everyone (neither the book nor the reality it reflects). The book is
written from the perspective of people who are smack in the middle of this new,
unhooked, unscripted maelstrom of love, sex, and disillusionment. It captures
how things are for a great number of people, not what many would say is ideal. It
is insightful and irreverent. Ansari is, after all, a comedian with the
bluntness of those who work the clubs.
Questions around how people
search for, and find, partners are part of an entire field of study about matching
problems. There is a need to match people to jobs, schools, and mates. From a
societal level, there is benefit in maximizing the number of people who match
with their best option. Individuals, of course, desire to make the best matches
they can in order to increase their odds of personal happiness, fulfillment, and
meaning.
You are likely familiar with a
myriad of services that solve matching problems of less importance than the
search for a mate. Uber, for example, matches drivers and riders. The algorithm
to do this is, of course, simpler than whatever it would take to increase people’s
odds of lifetime love and commitment in marriage. A 15-minute lift is different
from 60 years of driving together through life.
Ansari and Klinenberg describe massive
changes since decades past in how people search for mates (or dates, or just
sex). In mate selection, people have gone from choosing among two or three options
in their neighborhood or apartment building to trying to search through and
cope with the awareness of a myriad of options, thanks to advances in the
digital realm.
Searches are likely to fall short
of leading to good matches when people search too little or too much. What
strategy is just right? It’s very hard to know but the dilemmas involved lead to insights on how to have a reasonable perspective about it all.
The Train Station Problem
Samantha (Sam, for short) is
searching. She wants to find her soul-mate. I don’t merely mean that she wants a
mate who shares the deeper beliefs of her soul;[i]
she believes there is this one perfect partner out there for her—someone who would complete her in
ways far beyond being merely good, reliable, and committed. This is not an
unusual goal in modern-day mate searching,[ii]
and it complicates things quite a bit.
Eli
Finkel and colleagues have described the changing standards that guide our
search for mates: “Throughout American history, the fundamental purpose of
marriage has shifted from (a) helping spouses meet their basic economic and
political needs to (b) helping them meet their intimacy and passion needs to
(c) helping them meet their autonomy and personal-growth needs.”[iii]
Finkel and his coauthors argue that this expectation leads to average marriages
being less happy while a small number of marriages that can satisfy the
expectations for personal fulfillment may be happier than the “best marriages in
earlier eras.” Sam wants that.
Samantha is acutely aware of her dilemma.
She has no foolproof way of knowing where to find “the one” or how to know for
sure who is “the one” when she meets him. Metaphorically, Sam is in a train
station trying to figure out which train to get on and stay on for the ride of
her life.
Sam has checked out five trains. Those
“checks” ranged from having a brief coffee meet-up with one guy she met online
to being deeply involved with the last guy, whom she dated for 16 months before
eventually deciding he was not the one. That’s a long train ride, and it left Sam
further down the tracks at another station. Now she’s worried that she might
have missed the best option during those 16 months. Serious involvement often
has opportunity costs, you know. Still, she is a believing person, and she
takes comfort in a faith that God will not allow her to miss the right train. Still,
her belief in destiny is balanced by her belief that the odds are decreasing as
time marches on.
Sam wonders if the perfect train has
yet to appear at her station or if she’s missed it already. Her fear of missing
the best train is palpable and even paralyzing. “What if I make the wrong
choice?” “Am I even on the right rail line?” “How long do I stay on one train
before I get off if I am not sure it’s the one?” “What if I leave one train and
then, later, realize it was the best train for me?” A lot of the time Sam feels
like curling up on a bench and just letting all the trains roll through the
station without her even looking up.
Heuristics for Matching
Psychologists Peter Todd and
Geoffrey Miller wrote about mate searching in a respected volume on simple
heuristics for making the best decisions under various conditions of
uncertainty. [iv]
A heuristic is a mental short-cut that simplifies decision-making in order to
achieve what is often a good-enough solution where the costs of further effort
and time are unlikely to be worth the gain.
Todd and Miller describe some of
the history of attempts to come up with the best heuristic to solve searching
and matching problems in mate selection. They get to the heart of Samantha’s
anxiety, described above, which is the “uncertainty that the next prospect that
one encounters might be far superior to the best seen so far.”[v]
That is, she fears that once she makes a choice, the next train into the
station would be the one.
Todd and Miller note that if you could
know in advance the number of options you’d get to consider in choosing a mate,
you could use a guideline that a number of studies suggest yields the highest
likelihood of the best outcome. The rule is to select the best option that
appears after you have considered 37 percent of the options. You can see why
knowing the total number is important here, because otherwise, you’d have no
way to even guess when you’ll hit that 37 percent point. Todd and Miller
explain some of the arcane history of this decision rule, and they do a
particularly nice job of describing the necessary assumptions for such
heuristics to work.
Suppose Sam is going to have 10
trains to consider in her life. By this rule, she should check out the first four
but not choose any of them. Those poor guys don’t even know that they have no
chance. Sam is tough and she’s working the rule. But starting with number five,
Sam is ready to pick the first one that is better than any of the four she’s
seen so far. If the best option of all was in that first four, that’s pretty
sad. This may be, by the way, why people intuitively favor monitoring past
partners through social media; it’s become easy to do, and some people clearly
believe that it increases their odds of recalling a train (if it’s available)
back to the station.
Let’s suppose for a moment that
the 37 percent rule is pretty good for selecting a mate. (I personally prefer a strategy that’s a bit broader.) As Ansari and Klinenberg
argue, part of the problem for today’s young adults is that they are trying to
cope with an awareness of a truly countless number of potential partners. While
not actually true, a young adult today might think that the 37 percent rule means
checking out hundreds or thousands of potential mates. That’s going to take
some time, even in Grand Central Station.
Let’s bring sex into the
equation. If a person believes that he or she needs to check out a lot of
partners, including testing for sexual compatibility, that’s going to add up to
a lot of sexual partners before settling down in marriage. That strategy has
numerous risks which I will not enumerate here. Galena Rhoades and I find that
the median number of sexual partners emerging adults have before settling on a
mate is around five or six, and that having sex with others in addition to the
person one marries is associated (although, modestly) with lower marital
quality.[vi]
In a prior post, we attempted to explain why there could be something
causal in that, net of all the risks a person already may have in their
demographic background and life history.
Todd and Miller don’t leave us at
the 37 percent rule. They note that, even where all the assumptions are met, it
only leads to the best solution 37 percent of the time. Their main focus is to
make a mathematical case for “satisficing,” or accepting an option that meets a
reasonable level of expectation.[vii]
For example, they argue that a 14 percent search rule (instead of 37 percent) gets
83 percent of people in the top 10 percent of their options. Contrast that with
the people seeking absolute perfection, who may end up searching so long that
they leave behind better options before finally settling on the last train to
Clarksville.
The Misery of Searching for Your Perfect Soul Mate
As
Todd and Miller describe, Frey and Eichenberger (1996) argued that people do
not search adequately for a mate.[viii]
The distinguished sociologist Norval Glenn also made this point in a chapter
published in 2002.[ix] There are many causes of
poor searches. One of Glenn’s growing concerns was about how “premature
entanglement” was common and could foreclose adequate search for good matches. Norval
and I had a wonderful talk about these ideas over dinner in 2000. This was just
after I had started thinking a lot about the inertia problem with cohabitation. We both thought that a lot of
people were increasing their odds of taking the wrong train when they did not
have to do so. Thus, while there is increased freedom of choice and a growing availability
of tools for searching, these factors may be offset by the growing trend toward
sliding through relationship transitions in ways that lead to giving up options
before making a choice.[x]
You
don’t have to stay on a train for miles and miles to get a good sense for it.
Many do, however, owing to the ease of entry into cohabitation. People slide
into cohabiting,[xi] which rapidly escalates
inertia in the form of constraints; constraints make it more likely one will
stay in a relationship regardless of dedication to it.[xii]
Of course, many others are foregoing serious romantic involvement altogether,
being somewhat paralyzed by the fear of making the wrong choice. Why would that
be? While there are more tools than ever before that could be employed to
search for and sort into good matches, the expectations for marriage are also higher
than ever. The increasing availability of tools for searching might merely increase
fears of failing to find perfection; the quest may now appear both more
possible and impossible at the same time.
Despite
concerns in the late 1990s about inadequate search, I believe the changes Ansari
and Klinenberg document are real, and that in just the past 15 years, people
may have started to err in the direction of searching endlessly rather than searching
too little. Of course, an endless search for the perfect mate is also, in a very
real way, inadequate. Ansari and Klinenberg call attention to the work of
psychologist Barry Schwartz (The Paradox of Choice), who has written lucidly about the dilemmas of having too
many options. This argument by Schwartz that they recount is brilliant.
By Schwartz’s logic, we are probably
looking for “the best” and, in fact, we are looking for our soul mates too. Is
this possible to find? “How many people do you need to see before you know
you’ve found the best?” Schwartz asked. “The answer is every damn person there
is. How else do you know it’s the best? If you’re looking for the best, this is
a recipe for complete misery.”
That’s a whole lot of train tickets. Schwartz points out that the very belief that you can find the perfect match at the end of a search sets you up to think there must always be something better—an option that you’d not seen or found yet—and this makes people less happy with what they eventually choose.
Commitment is
making a choice to give up other choices. That’s the deal. Believing that you
could have found perfection—if you’d only searched a little more—will make it
harder to commit to, invest in, and be happy with the person you married.
[i]
Wilcox, W. B., and Wolfinger, N. H. (2015). Soul Mates: Religion, sex, love, and marriage among African Americans and Latinos. New York: Oxford
University Press.
[ii] Even 15 years ago, most emerging adults believed that when it came to selecting a mate, it was most important that their spouse be their “soul mate.” Popenoe, D., and Whitehead, B. D. (2001). Who wants to marry a soul mate? In D. Popenoe & B. D. Whitehead, The State of Our Unions: The Social Health of Marriage in America (pp. 6 - 16). Piscataway, NJ: National Marriage Project.
[iii] Finkel, E. J., Cheung, E. O., Emery, L. F., Carswell, K. L., and Larson, G. M. (2015). The suffocation model: Why marriage in American is becoming an all-or-nothing institution. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(3), 283-244. doi: 10.1177/0963721415569274
[iv] Todd, P. M., and Miller, G. F. (1999). From Pride and Prejudice to Persuasion: Satisficing in Mate Search. In G. Gigerenzer, P. M. Todd, and The ABC Research Group (Eds.), Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart (pp. 287-308). New York: Oxford University Press.
[v] Ibid.
[vi] Rhoades, G. K., and Stanley, S. M. (2014). Before “I Do”: What do premarital experiences have to do with marital quality among today’s young adults? Charlottesville, VA: National Marriage Project.
[vii] Simon, H. A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 1-19.
[viii] Frey, B. S., and Eichenberger, R. (1996). Marriage paradoxes. Rationality and Society, 8(2), 187-206.
[ix] Glenn, N. D. (2002). A plea for greater concern about the quality of marital matching. In A. J. Hawkins, L. D. Wardle, and D. O. Coolidge (Eds.), Revitalizing the institution of marriage for the twenty-first century: An agenda for strengthening marriage (pp. 45-58). Westport, CT: Praeger.
[x] For more on this theme of giving up options before making a choice, you can read this piece or listen to this 24 minute talk I gave earlier this year.
[xi] Stanley, S. M., Rhoades, G. K., and Markman, H. J. (2006). Sliding vs. Deciding: Inertia and the premarital cohabitation effect. Family Relations, 55, 499-509.
[xii] Rhoades, G. K., Stanley, S. M., and Markman, H. J. (2012). The impact of the transition to cohabitation on relationship functioning: Cross-sectional and longitudinal findings. Journal of Family Psychology, 26(3), 348-358.; Rhoades, G. K., Stanley, S. M., and Markman, H. J. (2010). Should I stay or should I go? Predicting dating relationship stability from four aspects of commitment. Journal of Family Psychology, 24(5), 543-550.
[ii] Even 15 years ago, most emerging adults believed that when it came to selecting a mate, it was most important that their spouse be their “soul mate.” Popenoe, D., and Whitehead, B. D. (2001). Who wants to marry a soul mate? In D. Popenoe & B. D. Whitehead, The State of Our Unions: The Social Health of Marriage in America (pp. 6 - 16). Piscataway, NJ: National Marriage Project.
[iii] Finkel, E. J., Cheung, E. O., Emery, L. F., Carswell, K. L., and Larson, G. M. (2015). The suffocation model: Why marriage in American is becoming an all-or-nothing institution. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(3), 283-244. doi: 10.1177/0963721415569274
[iv] Todd, P. M., and Miller, G. F. (1999). From Pride and Prejudice to Persuasion: Satisficing in Mate Search. In G. Gigerenzer, P. M. Todd, and The ABC Research Group (Eds.), Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart (pp. 287-308). New York: Oxford University Press.
[v] Ibid.
[vi] Rhoades, G. K., and Stanley, S. M. (2014). Before “I Do”: What do premarital experiences have to do with marital quality among today’s young adults? Charlottesville, VA: National Marriage Project.
[vii] Simon, H. A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 1-19.
[viii] Frey, B. S., and Eichenberger, R. (1996). Marriage paradoxes. Rationality and Society, 8(2), 187-206.
[ix] Glenn, N. D. (2002). A plea for greater concern about the quality of marital matching. In A. J. Hawkins, L. D. Wardle, and D. O. Coolidge (Eds.), Revitalizing the institution of marriage for the twenty-first century: An agenda for strengthening marriage (pp. 45-58). Westport, CT: Praeger.
[x] For more on this theme of giving up options before making a choice, you can read this piece or listen to this 24 minute talk I gave earlier this year.
[xi] Stanley, S. M., Rhoades, G. K., and Markman, H. J. (2006). Sliding vs. Deciding: Inertia and the premarital cohabitation effect. Family Relations, 55, 499-509.
[xii] Rhoades, G. K., Stanley, S. M., and Markman, H. J. (2012). The impact of the transition to cohabitation on relationship functioning: Cross-sectional and longitudinal findings. Journal of Family Psychology, 26(3), 348-358.; Rhoades, G. K., Stanley, S. M., and Markman, H. J. (2010). Should I stay or should I go? Predicting dating relationship stability from four aspects of commitment. Journal of Family Psychology, 24(5), 543-550.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)